Cognitive Brain Research 4:15-25, 1996
Tonic, Phasic, and Transient EEG Correlates of Auditory Awareness in Drowsiness
Institute for Neural Computation
University of California San Diego
and The Salk Institute
La Jolla CA 92037 and
858 453 4100 x1455
Download PDF Download a (248k) gzipped Postscript (.ps.gz) version of this article.
Email authors for a reprintKeywords: EEG sleep auditory perception theta gamma spindle ERP
Summary
During drowsiness, human performance in responding to above-threshold auditory targets tends to vary irregularly over periods of 4 minutes and longer. These performance fluctuations are accompanied by distinct changes in the frequency spectrum of the electroencephalogram (EEG) on three time scales:
(1) During minute-scale and longer periods of intermittent responding, mean activity levels in the (< 4 Hz) delta and (4-6 Hz) theta bands, and at the sleep spindle frequency (14 Hz) are higher than during alert performance.
(2) In most subjects, 4-6 Hz theta EEG activity begins to increase, and gamma band activity above 35 Hz begins to decrease, about 10 s before presentations of undetected targets, while before detected targets, 4-6 Hz amplitude decreases and gamma band amplitude increases. Both these amplitude differences last 15-20 s and occur in parallel with event-related cycles in target detection probability. In the same periods, alpha and sleep-spindle frequency amplitudes also show prominent 15-20 s cycles, but these are not phase locked to performance cycles.
(3) A second or longer after undetected targets, amplitude at intermediate (10-25 Hz) frequencies decreases briefly, while detected targets are followed by a transient amplitude increase in the same latency and frequency range.
Introduction
Methods and Materials
Results
Discussion
Conclusions
Acknowledgements
References
Your email comments/questions? Other publications on alertness
Introduction
Human subjects' ability to sustain their initial level of performance during continuous auditory or visual monitoring tasks is limited. After only a few minutes on task, particularly in low-arousal environments, performance of subjects in auditory or visual monitoring tasks often includes periods of intermittent failures to respond to above-threshold targets, alternating with periods of consistent responding. Cycles of relatively good and poor performance tend to last four minutes or longer[4, 18] and are experienced by subjects as alternating waves of drowsiness and alertness[23]. Under sustained low-arousal conditions or when subjects are fatigued, these drowsy periods may progress into sleep episodes. Minute-scale changes in the probability of detection in above-threshold signals during auditory vigilance tasks are accompanied by simultaneous shifts in spectral amplitude of several relatively narrow EEG frequency bands[18], these changes appearing to index the action of brain mechanisms involved in modulating arousal and/or auditory information processing in and near to sleep[26].
One hallmark of the transition from drowsiness to sleep is the appearance of EEG sleep spindles, intermittent oscillatory thalamocortical bursts near 14 Hz[7, 25]. Some early EEG studies of drowsiness also associated lapses in auditory detection with increased EEG amplitude in the theta range (4-7 Hz)[2, 6, 23]. In this study, we explore whether lapses in auditory detection during periods of intermittent detection performance are accompanied by changes in the amplitude of spontaneous EEG activity, and, in particular, whether individual failures to detect auditory target stimuli are more closely associated with increased activity at the sleep spindling frequency or with theta bursts. Finally, we report that different transient perturbations in the EEG spectrum follow presentation of detected and undetected auditory targets and non-target tones.
Materials and Methods
Concurrent EEG and behavioral data were collected for the purpose of developing an objective method of monitoring the alertness of operators of complex systems[13, 17, 18]. Fifteen young adult volunteers participated in five half-hour sessions each, during which they pushed one button whenever they detected an above-threshold auditory target stimulus (a brief increase in the level of continuous background noise), and another button whenever they detected a visual pattern on a computer screen in front of them (a slowly-descending line of dots in visual noise).
Stimuli
Auditory targets consisted of 300 ms increases in the intensity of a 62 dB white noise background (150 ms rise, 110 ms fall), presented 6 dB above their threshold of detectibility at pseudorandom time intervals producing a mean rate of 10/min. Brief (50 ms) non-target probe tones of two frequencies (20% 568 Hz, 80% 1098 Hz) were interspersed between the target noise bursts at 2-4 s intervals. Visual targets were vertical lines of dots presented in a slowly descending (2 lines/sec) visual noise 'waterfall' pattern at a mean rate of 1/min. Sessions were conducted in a small, warm, and dimly-lit experimental chamber. Visual task data will be reported elsewhere.
Data Collection
EEG was collected from two electrodes at the vertex and posterior midline (Cz and Pz/Oz, midway between Pz and Oz), referred to the right mastoid, at a sampling rate of 312.5 Hz after analogue filtering (Grass Model 12, bandpass 0.1-100 Hz, 12 dB/octave). Channel locations were selected as likely to contain independent alertness information on the basis of previous studies[18]. Bipolar vertical and horizontal electrooculogram (EOG) data were also recorded for use in artifact rejection. Data windows containing potential deviations from baseline of more than 75 uV were rejected from analysis. Three sessions each from ten subjects containing sufficient (mean +/- s.d., 56+/-26) response lapses were used in the analysis.
Local Error Rate
A time-varying, causal behavioral index of alertness, local error rate, was defined by computing a moving average probability of detection of auditory targets smoothed using a 95-s wide exponential moving window (90% down 95 s back) whose width and shape were chosen to increase smoothness of the resulting measure while decreasing the (23 s) time lag from the leading edge of the window to its center of gravity. Time-varying EEG spectra with 0.6 Hz resolution were computed at 1.64-s intervals using running median averages of FFT transforms of Hanning-windowed 0.82-s data epochs with 50% overlap (zero-padded to 512 points / 1.64 s). The median filtering window spanned seven overlapping 0.82-s data windows, encompassing 3.28 s of data. The four median-filtered spectral estimates in each 1.64-s interval were then averaged.
Error-Sorted Spectra
Changes in the EEG spectrum as a function of error rate were computed by separately averaging log spectra in 4-second epochs preceding detected (hit) and undetected (lapse) target presentations, sorting these spectra according to the prevailing error rate, and then smoothing the results using a Hanning window (encompassing a 30% error-rate range) which was moved through the error-sorted data in 5% error-rate steps. The mean log spectrum during the first 2 min of error-free performance was then subtracted from each spectral trace, yielding hit- and lapse-related error-sorted spectra revealing mean changes in the EEG spectrum accompanying different levels of performance (see Fig. 1).
Spectral Perturbations
The time course of event-related changes in the amplitude of oscillatory EEG activity time locked, but not necessarily phase locked, to experimental events can be measured using normalized time-frequency transforms, previously called event-related spectral perturbations (ERSPs)[16]. Averages of time-frequency transforms of EEG epochs time locked to experimental events can measure event-related modulations of EEG amplitude at a wide range of frequencies and time scales (tenths of seconds[12] to minutes or longer[9, 18]). Time- and frequency-specific ERSP features are largely independent of concurrent features of time-domain response averages (event-related potentials or ERPs). ERPs contain potential deviations occurring at fixed times and in fixed phases relative to experimental events, while ERSPs record changes in the amplitude of EEG oscillations irrespective of their phase at event onsets. Here, ERSPs time locked to hit and lapse target noise bursts, and to non-target tones, were computed by averaging logarithmic time-frequency amplitude transforms of 54-s spectral epochs surrounding stimulus onsets. Two types of normalization were applied (see Fig. 2).
Results
Error-Sorted Spectra
During periods of poor detection performance, the mean error-sorted EEG spectrum at the vertex prior to lapses
Figure 1a. Grand mean error-sorted spectra at the vertex (Cz). Mean EEG log spectra during 4 second intervals preceding target presentations were sorted by local error rate (see text) and normalized by subtracting the mean log spectrum of the first two minutes of alert zero-error performance in each session. Grand means of data from 3 sessions for each of 10 subjects. Contour level intervals: 1 dB. (a) Error-sorted spectrum for periods preceding detected targets (hits) shows error rate-related increases in low-frequency (< 4 Hz) and near-14 Hz activity.
Fig. 1a contains two prominent maxima -- relative increases in EEG amplitude near 5 Hz and 14 Hz, plus a smaller increase at frequencies below 5 Hz. At high error rates, mean amplitude near 10 Hz at site Pz/Oz also decreased in most subjects below its eyes-open baseline level. The 14-Hz peak appears only when error rate is above 80%, consistent with the emergence of 14-Hz spindles at sleep onset[7]). At high error rates, the error-sorted spectrum at Cz prior to hits
Figure 1b. Error-sorted spectrum preceding undetected targets (lapses) has another peak near 5 Hz.
Fig. 1b also contain a peak near 14 Hz, but has no peak near 5 Hz. During high error-rate periods, 14-Hz amplitude is nearly equally as elevated before lapses as before hits, while in our 10 subjects mean amplitude near 5 Hz is significantly larger before lapses at all non-zero error rate levels (F(1,9) > 22.9, p < 0.001). The (lapse minus hit) difference between the two error-sorted spectra
Figure 1c. Difference between (1a) and (1b) (prelapse - prehit) showing, during high error-rate periods, a relative increase in near 5-Hz activity before lapses, accompanied by a relative decrease at higher frequencies.
Fig. 1c, therefore, contains a circa 5-Hz positivity at all error rates accompanied, at high error rates, by a general decrease in activity above 8 Hz -- but no feature at 14 Hz.
Spectral Perturbations
Figure 2a. Event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) transforms at Cz of 56-s epochs centered on targets presented when local error rate was between 20% and 80%. Vertical scale is log amplitude (in dB) relative to baseline. Grand means of 30 sessions from 10 subjects. Contour levels: in (a), 1 dB in (a); in (b-d), 4 and 8 s.d. (in the first 8 s of the plotted ERSP epoch). (a) Alert-normalized ERSP time locked to undetected targets (lapses). Note the phasic EEG amplitude increase near 5 Hz superimposed on tonically increased (1-6 Hz) slow activity.
Fig. 2a shows the mean changes in the EEG log spectrum at Cz before, during, and after undetected noise burst targets (lapses) presented during periods of intermittent (20-80% error rate) detection performance in the same 30 sessions. Single-session ERSPs were normalized by subtracting the mean log spectrum during the first two minutes of error-free performance, to show both the tonic EEG spectral changes associated with periods of non-zero (20-80%) error rate, and phasic EEG changes associated with detection lapses during these periods. The figure shows the expected tonic amplitude increase at 5 Hz and below, and also a phasic increase near 5 Hz beginning several seconds before delivery of undetected targets.
To study this and other features of Fig. 2a in more detail, time-frequency transforms of data epochs time locked to undetected (lapse) target presentations were individually normalized by subtracting a pre-stimulus baseline log spectral estimate from each spectral trace. The baseline estimate was the mean log spectrum in an exponential window with leading edge 10 s before, and its (90%-down) trailing edge 105 s before target onset. The resulting normalized transforms, containing time-varying spectral fluctuations relative to the mean spectrum in the preceding 105-s period, were then averaged and their grand mean across subjects and sessions was calculated. (Note that baseline periods included an average of 17 previous target and 34 non-target tone presentations). Target hit-related ERSP averages were computed by the same procedure.
width=499 height=400 Figure 2b. Single-trial baseline normalized ERSP transform of the lapse-related data (see text). Note the prestimulus increase near 5 Hz and (less noticeable) decrease near 40 Hz, and the brief suppression of activity at intermediate frequencies after the stimulus.
Figure 2c. Single-trial normalized ERSP for epochs surrounding detected targets (hits). Note the anticipatory relative decrease in amplitude near 5 Hz, the simultaneous increase above 30 Hz, and the stimulus-induced ridge of increased 10-20+ Hz activity.Lapse-related (Fig. 2b) and hit-related (Fig. 2c) ERSPs differ significantly in three ways: (1) the mean increase (or decrease) in 5-Hz activity surrounding target presentation; (2) the opposing amplitude decrease (or increase) above 35 Hz; (3) the trough (or ridge) amplitude perturbation at 10-25 Hz appearing after hit (or lapse) target presentations, respectively, with a frequency extremum at 11 Hz.
Figure 2d. Single-trial normalized (lapse - hit) difference between (b) and (c), containing features common to both transforms.
As the hit and lapse target ERSPs in Figs. 2b and 2c are negatively correlated (r = -0.89), their difference (Fig. 2d) emphasizes their common features.
Fig. 2e shows the time course of F(1,18)-statistics for hit/lapse differences at each time point in 20 sessions (two sessions containing the most lapses from each of the 10 subjects). The figure shows the time course of this statistic for response-related log amplitude differences in theta, alpha, and gamma frequency bands. Whereas these differences in theta and gamma band activity begin nearly 10 s before the stimulus, a response-related difference in alpha band amplitudes appears only after stimulus onset.
Performance Cycles
Next, we asked whether the phasic increases in 5-Hz activity before and after lapses were accompanied by transient changes in target detection probability. As might be expected, the probability of subjects of detecting targets presented immediately before and after detected and undetected targets, respectively, differed from one another and from the local error-rate baseline. Surprisingly, both these performance-related differences had similar and well-defined time courses, which were quantified as the probability of a lapse in a 1.64-s time window moved through the 58-s ERSP epoch in 0.41-s steps.
Figure 3a. Time course of event-related error rate and relative 4-6 Hz EEG log amplitude at Pz/Oz (each smoothed in a 1.64-s moving window) before and after the same detected (hit) and undetected (lapse) targets as in Fig. 2a. (a) Grand means for 30 sessions from 10 subjects. Note divergence of all four measures from 10 s before to 10 s after target presentations.
Fig. 3a shows results at site Pz/Oz; results at Cz were similar. In this and following figures showing event-related error rate trends, the steepness and width of the error rate extrema at time zero reflect the 1.64-s smoothing width used and the minimum 1-s interval between target presentations. Ten seconds after hits, both error rate and 4-6 Hz amplitude settled at somewhat lower levels than their pre-stimulus baselines, presumably reflecting a net arousing effect of target detections and/or responses.
Different sessions from the same subject gave highly similar error-rate perturbations (Fig. 4a below) which closely parallelled the time course of the 4-6 Hz EEG amplitude trends in group and most single subject data (Figs. 3bc).
Figure 3b. Time course of relative 4-6 Hz log amplitude and error rate in one of the seven subjects showing the error-rate trend seen in the grand mean (Fig 3a).
Figure 3c. Same for one of three subjects not showing 15-20 s EEG and error-rate cycles (see Fig. 4a).
Figure 4a. Time course of differences in response-related error-rate time series at site Pz/Oz. Differences between lapse and hit event-related records (like those shown in Fig. 3) are shown smoothed with a 1.64-s moving window. Top traces: grand means. Lower traces: individual sessions, with pairs of sessions from the same subject superimposed. Subjects data are arranged in the same order in both panels, with data from the three subjects showing no evidence of 15-20 s performance cycles marked with asterisks. Subject codes are listed on the left of each panel.
Fig. 4a shows differences between the time course of error rate changes time locked to lapses and hits respectively. The top trace shows the grand mean error-rate difference; the traces below it are for 20 single sessions, with pairs of sessions from the same subject superimposed.
Figure 4b. Time course of differences in response-related 4-6 Hz amplitude time series at site Pz/Oz.
Fig. 4b shows the time course of 4-6 Hz amplitude differences in the same sessions. Note the strong within-subject replicability of both the performance and amplitude cycles, and the stable between-subject differences. The top three trace pairs in each panel are from three subjects who do not show response-related 15-20 s performance cycles. The 4-6 Hz theta amplitude trends for at least two of these three subjects (Fig 4b, top two pairs) likewise contain no 15-20 s cycles.
Discussion
Our results show that variations in sustained auditory detection performance have distinct EEG spectral correlates on three time-scales, here categorized as tonic changes, phasic cycles, and transient perturbations.
Tonic Changes
During minute-scale and longer periods of intermittent responding, mean EEG amplitudes in the delta (< 4 Hz), theta (4-6 Hz), and sleep-spindle (near 14 Hz) bands tend to increase at Cz and at Oz/Pz. During these periods, delta and 14-Hz amplitudes are equally as large before hits as before lapses , while 4-6 Hz activity is significantly larger before lapses than before hits.
During sleep, behavioral and brain responsiveness to sounds is decreased, and communication of afferent auditory information between the thalamus and auditory cortex is restricted or gated[25, 26]. Although a tonic increase in activity at the human sleep spindle frequency (near 14 Hz)[7] accompanies sustained decrements in detection probability, 14-Hz amplitude is not related to the detection of individual targets Fig. 1c). Our analyses do not determine what portion of the 14-Hz peaks in Fig. 1a and 1b reflect the appearance of classic sleep spindles. However, the lack of a 14-Hz feature in Fig. 1c strongly suggests that sleep spindles, hallmarks of transition to slow-wave sleep, cannot be directly involved in intermittent gating of sensory information during drowsiness, although they may have a role in producing tonic changes in auditory sensitivity during sleep onset[25].
The most important conclusion to be drawn from the error-sorted spectra (Fig. 1) is that during periods of intermittent performance, individual detection lapses are associated with increased activity near 5 Hz, not with 14-Hz sleep spindles or increased delta activity. ERSP (Fig. 2) and event-related error-rate (Fig. 3) analyses demonstrate that the observed 4-6 Hz differences arise from phase-locking of 15-20 s EEG and performance cycles.
Phasic Cycles
During periods of inconsistent responding, seven of our ten subjects show 15-20 s cycles in detection probability, closely paralleled by opposing changes in EEG amplitudes near 5 Hz and above 35 Hz. In accord with claims of early EEG studies, undetected targets are preceded by increased activity near 5 Hz, while in our experiments EEG activity above 35 Hz tends to increase prior to detected targets.
Second-order spectral analyses of amplitude variance at each EEG frequency (not shown), applied to the same epochs used to compute the ERSPs (Fig. 2), revealed a peak cycle length between 15 and 20 s per cycle over a wide range of EEG frequencies, consistent with other reports[19]. The 15-20 s theta and gamma perturbations in Fig. 3a, then, may result from mutual phase locking of spontaneous amplitude fluctuations at these frequencies, uncorrelated with 15-20 s fluctuations at lower (< 4 Hz) and intermediate frequencies (10-25 Hz). The fact that the theta-band amplitude cycles are also time locked to performance cycles (Fig. 3a) suggests that intermittent gating of auditory information transmission during drowsiness may be intimately connected to 4-6 Hz activity.
While potential neurophysiological mechanisms for this phenomena have been discussed[25, 26], the behavioral and physiological distinction we report here between spindling-frequency activity and the sleep-related theta rhythm does not seem to have been observed in animal brains. [NOTE: However, 20 s cycles in EEG amplitude have been shown to be synchronized to sleep onset in neocortex of rats (Oakson & Steriade, 1983, Fig. 3]. Note that we cannot rule out possibilities that some or all lapses in these experiments arose from subjects sleep-related inability to recall the significance of the tones, or from an inability to press the response button, although in our own experience as subject (tpj) and pilot subject (sm), all three dimensions of awareness (auditory, situational, and somatic) seemed to covary.
This is not the first report that changes in theta and gamma band EEG amplitudes are linked to changes in attention and awareness[8, 11]. Intermittent vigilance decrements similar to the 15-20 s cycles we report here have recently been reported in monkeys performing a continuous visual task, and have been correlated with cyclic decreases in locus coeruleus activity[21]. Conversely, noradrenergic locus coeruleus activation is known to produce behavioral alerting, accompanied by a rapid shift of the cortical EEG spectrum from low to high frequencies[27]. Possibly, a similar modulator may underlie the 15-20 s performance and EEG fluctuations seen both in quietly resting[19] and here in drowsy humans. More than one brain arousal subsystem may also be involved in producing them[25].
Our observed pre-stimulus increase in theta-band EEG prior to lapses in these experiments confirms and further quantifies early reports[6], most notably a 1962 study of Williams et al.28 in which this phenomenon was seen in EEG tracings of some subjects after 30 hours of sleep deprivation. In other early EEG studies, "low-level irregular" theta activity was associated with self-reports of daydreaming and "floating" sensations typical of the hypnagogic period preceding sleep onset[23]. Most recent studies of theta band EEG, however, have focussed on dissimilar phenomena associated with arousal and attention, including rhythmic slow activity (RSA) in the septohippocampal and related system appearing in animals during volitional movements[14] and frontal midline theta EEG in humans associated with mental effort[10].
The increased gamma-band activity preceding detected targets in our data is compatible with hypotheses that transient phase correlations among local gamma band oscillations in the cortex and thalamus may help support allocation of perceptual attention[5, 8, 15, 22]. Although we cannot rule out possible contributions of muscle activity to changes in gamma band activity prior to detected or undetected targets, muscle activity normally contains frequencies below 35 Hz, the lower edge of our performance-related gamma-band difference, suggesting that the phasic gamma band phenomenon shown in Fig. 2 arises predominately from performance-related changes in brain activity.
Transient Perturbations
Successfully detected auditory targets are followed by a brief increase in 10-25 Hz EEG activity beginning a second or more after stimulus onset, while undetected targets are followed by a brief relative amplitude decrease in the same time and frequency range.
The appearance of 10-25 Hz spectral perturbations following (but not preceding) undetected (Fig. 2b) as well as detected (Fig. 2c) targets implies that the brain system responsible for these EEG modulations also detects lapse targets, to which subjects do not respond. ERSPs (not shown) time locked to onsets of the frequent (16/min) non-target tones in these sessions contained no comparable response feature, implying that the EEG modulator responsible for the 10-25 Hz response can also distinguish target noise bursts from non-target tones, even though the non-target tones in these experiments were perceptually more salient than the relatively faint target noise bursts. Similarly, in auditory experiments reported earlier[16], a ridge of augmented 11-19 Hz activity induced by very brief target tone pips was found to be twice as large that induced by intense (84 dB SL), long-lasting (1 s) non-target tones.
Note that this attention-related 10-25 Hz perturbation appears much later than attention-related features of the auditory ERP, such as the P300, but closer in latency to attention-related autonomic responses such as the evoked pupillary response1. In general, the magnitudes of ERSP features (0.5 - 2 dB, 5 - 25%) in this and previously-reported experiments are comparable in size to task-related differences in local brain metabolism reported in many PET and fMRI brain studies[20, 24], suggesting a possible relation between EEG spectral perturbations and the size of local brain blood-flow changes.
Conclusions
Concurrent activity on a wide range of time scales is a basic fact of brain physiology[3]. Our results show that during periods of impaired performance on an auditory vigilance task, at least three time scales characterize the dynamics of variations in auditory detection performance and the EEG spectrum. Overall, changes in error rate in auditory detection are dominated by irregular waves of performance changes lasting four minutes and longer[4, 18]. Within these, phasic 15-20 s cycles in detection performance are phase locked to counterbalanced changes in EEG amplitudes near 5 Hz and above 35 Hz, respectively. Finally, auditory stimuli induce transient and variable perturbations in the EEG amplitude spectrum which can be observed by averaging normalized time-frequency transforms (ERSPs) of event-related data epochs. In particular, faint target noise bursts (but not more salient non-target tones) in our experiments induce transient time locked perturbations in mean EEG amplitudes at intermediate frequencies (10-25 Hz), whose sign (positive or negative) depends on whether or not the subject responds to the target. Further understanding of characteristic time and frequency scales involved in cognitive brain processing during sleep and waking may lead to a deeper appreciation of the organization of human consciousness and performance, and may also have practical applications in monitoring the alertness of operators of complex systems[13, 17].
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge the valuable contributions of F. Scot Elliott and Mark Postal in collecting and archiving the data, and thank Mark Inlow and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions, and Terry Sejnowski for technical support. This research was supported by work unit ONR.WR.30020(6429) from the Office of Naval Research.
Disclaimer. This work was supported by the Department of the Navy, Naval Medical Research and Development Command, Bethesda, Maryland under work unit ONR.WR.30020(6429).The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Department of Defense, nor the U.S. Government. Approved for release, distribution unlimited.
References
Return to Summary.
- Beatty, J., Phasic not tonic pupillary responses vary with auditory vigilance performance, Psychophysiology, 19 (1982) 167-172.
- Beatty, J., Greenberg, A., Deibler, W.P., and O'Hanlon J.F., Operant control of occipital theta rhythm affects performance in a radar monitoring task. Science, 183 (1974) 871-873.
- Churchland, P.S, .and Sejnowski, T.J., Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. Science, 242 (1988) 741-745.
- Conte, S., Ferlazzo, F., and Renzi, P., Ultradian rhythms of reaction times in performance in vigilance tasks. Biolog. Psychol., 399 (1995) 159-172.
- Crick. F., and Koch, C., Some reflections on visual awareness. Cold Spring Harbor Sympos. on Quant. Biol., 55 (1990) 953-962.
- Davis, H., Davis, P.A., Loomis, A.L., Harvey, E.N., and Hobart, G., Human brain potentials during the onset of sleep. J. Neurophysiol., 2 (1937) 24-37.
- Dijk, D-J., Hayes, B., and Czeisler, C.A., Dynamics of electroencephalographic sleep spindles and slow wave activity in men: effect of sleep deprivation. Brain Res., 626 (1993) 190-199.
- Freeman, W., Mass Action in the Nervous System, Academic Press, New York, 1975, 489 pp.
- Galambos, R., and Makeig, S., Dynamic changes in steady-state responses. In: H. Basar (Ed.), Dynamics of Sensory and Cognitive Processing by the Brain, Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1988, pp. 103-122.
- Inouye, T., Shimosako, K., Iyama, A., Matsumoto, Y., and Toi, S., Moving potential field of frontal midline theta activity during a mental task. Cog. Brain Res., 2 (1994) 87-92.
- Jahnsen, H., and Llinas, R., Electrophysiological properties of guinea-pig thalamic neurons: an in vitro study. J. Physiol. Lond., 349 (1984) 205-225.
- Jokeit, H., and Makeig, S., Different event-related patterns of gamma-band power in brain waves of fast- and slow-reacting subjects. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 91 (1994) 6339-6343.
- Jung, T-P., and Makeig, S., Predicting failures in auditory detection from changes in the EEG spectrum, Abstract, IEEE Soc. for Engin. in Med. and Biol. (1995).
- Leung, L-W.S., Lopes da Silva, F.H., and Wadman, W.J., Spectral characteristics of the hippocampal EEG in the freely moving rat. Electroencephalog. clin. Neurophysiolog., 54 (1982) 203-219.
- Lisman, J.E., and Idiart, M.A.P., Storage of 72 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science, 267 (1995) 1512-1516.
- Makeig, S., Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. Electroencephalog. clin. Neurophysiolog., 86 (1993) 283-293.
- Makeig, S. Elliott, F.S., and Postal M. First Demonstration of an Alertness Monitoring and Management System. Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, CA, 1993, Tech. Report 93-36.
- Makeig, S. and Inlow, M., Lapses in alertness: coherence of fluctuations in performance and EEG spectrum. Electroencephalog. clin. Neurophysiolog., 86 (1993) 23-35.
- Novak, P., Lepicovska, V., and Dostalek, C., Periodic amplitude modulation of EEG. Neuroscience Lett. 136 (1992) 213-215.
- Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Snyder, A.Z., and Raichle, M.E., Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. Science, 249 (1990) 1041-1044.
- Rajkowski, J., Kubiak, P., and Aston-Jones, G., Locus coeruleus activity in monkey: phasic and tonic changes are associated with altered vigilance. Brain Res. Bull., 35 (1994) 607-616.
- Rogeul, A., Bouyer, J.J., Dedet, L., and Debray, O. Fast somato-parietal rhythms during combined focal attention and immobility in baboon and squirrel monkey. Electroencephalog. clin. Neurophysiolog., 46 (1979) 310-319.
- Schacter, D.L. The hypnagogic state: a critical review of the literature. Psycholog. Bull., 83 (1976) 452-481.
- Schneider, W., Noll, D.C., and Cohen, J.D., Functional topographic mapping of the cortical ribbon in human vision with conventional MRI scanners. Nature, 365 (1993) 150-153.
- Steriade, M., Jones, E.G. and Llinas, R.R. Thalamic Oscillations and Signaling, Wiley, New York, 1990, 431 pp.
- Steriade, M., McCormick,D.A., and Sejnowski, T.J., Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science, 262 (1993) 679-688.
- Swick, D., Pineda, J.A., Schacher, S., and Foote, S.L., Locus coeruleus neuronal activity in awake monkeys: relationship to auditory P300-like potentials and spontaneous EEG. Exp. Brain Res., 101 (1994) 86-92.
- Williams, H.A., Granda, A., Jones, R., Lubin, A., and Armington, J., EEG frequency and finger pulse volume as predictors of reaction time during sleep loss. Electroencephalog. clin. Neurophysiolog., 14 (1962) 64-70.